You are sitting quietly, and suddenly your brain tunes out the world and
wanders to something else entirely — perhaps a recent experience, or an old
memory. You just had a daydream.
Yet despite the ubiquity of this experience, what is happening in the brain
while daydreaming is a question that has largely eluded neuroscientists.
Now, a study
in mice, published Dec. 13 in Nature, has
brought a team led by researchers at Harvard Medical School one step closer to
figuring it out.
The researchers tracked the activity of neurons in the visual cortex of the
brains of mice while the animals remained in a quiet waking state. They found
that occasionally these neurons fired in a pattern similar to one that occurred
when a mouse looked at an actual image, suggesting that the mouse was thinking
— or daydreaming — about the image. Moreover, the patterns of activity during a
mouse’s first few daydreams of the day predicted how the brain’s response to
the image would change over time.
The research provides tantalizing, if preliminary, evidence that daydreams
can shape the brain’s future response to what it sees. This causal relationship
needs to be confirmed in further research, the team cautioned, but the results
offer an intriguing clue that daydreams during quiet waking may play a role in
brain plasticity — the brain’s ability to remodel itself in response to new
experiences.
“We wanted to know how this daydreaming process occurred on a neurobiological level, and whether these moments of quiet reflection could be important for learning and memory,” said lead author Nghia Nguyen, a PhD student in neurobiology in the Blavatnik Institute at HMS.
An overlooked brain region
Scientists
have spent considerable time studying how neurons replay past events to form
memories and map the physical environment in the hippocampus, a seahorse-shaped
brain region that plays a key role in memory and spatial navigation.
By contrast, there has been little research on the replay of neurons in other
brain regions, including the visual cortex. Such efforts would provide valuable
insights about how visual memories are formed.
“My lab became
interested in whether we could record from enough neurons in the visual cortex
to understand what exactly the mouse is remembering — and then connect that
information to brain plasticity,” said senior author Mark
Andermann, professor of medicine at Beth Israel Deaconess
Medical Center, and professor of neurobiology at HMS.
During
the experiments, mice repeatedly looked at one of two images, shown here, with
one-minute breaks in between. The images were selected based on their ability
to elicit a strong response from neurons in the visual cortex. Video: Andermann
lab
In the new study, the
researchers repeatedly showed mice one of two images, each consisting of a
different checkerboard pattern of gray and dappled black and white squares.
Between images, the mice spent a minute looking at a gray screen. The team
simultaneously recorded activity from around 7,000 neurons in the visual
cortex.
The researchers found that when a mouse
looked at an image, the neurons fired in a specific pattern, and the patterns
were different enough to discern image one from image two. More important, when
a mouse looked at the gray screen between images, the neurons sometimes fired
in a similar, but not identical, pattern, as when the mouse looked at the
image, a sign that it was daydreaming about the image. These daydreams occurred
only when mice were relaxed, characterized by calm behavior and small pupils.
Unsurprisingly, mice daydreamed more about the most recent image — and they had more daydreams at the beginning of the day than at the end, when they had already seen each image dozens of times.
Between
images, mice spent a minute looking at a gray screen. During this time, neurons
in the visual cortex of the brain, shown here, occasionally fired in a pattern
similar to one seen when the mice were looking at an image, suggesting that
mice were daydreaming about the image. Video: Andermann lab
But what the researchers found next was completely unexpected.
Throughout the day, and across days, the activity patterns seen when the
mice looked at the images changed — what neuroscientists call “representational
drift.” Yet this drift wasn’t random. Over time, the patterns associated with
the images became even more different from each other, until each involved an
almost entirely separate set of neurons. Notably, the pattern seen during a
mouse’s first few daydreams about an image predicted what the pattern would
become when the mouse looked at the image later.
“There’s drift in how the brain responds to the same image over time, and
these early daydreams can predict where the drift is going,” Andermann said.
Finally, the researchers found that the visual cortex daydreams occurred at the same time as replay activity occurred in the hippocampus, suggesting that the two brain regions were communicating during these daydreams.
To sit, perchance to daydream
Based on the results of the study, the researches suspect that these
daydreams may be actively involved in brain plasticity.
“When you see two different images many times, it becomes important to
discriminate between them. Our findings suggest that daydreaming may guide this
process by steering the neural patterns associated with the two images away
from each other,” Nguyen said, while noting that this relationship needs to be
confirmed.
Nguyen added that learning to differentiate between the images should help
the mouse respond to each image with more specificity in the future.
These
observations align with a growing body of evidence in rodents
and humans that entering a state of quiet wakefulness after
an experience can improve learning and memory.
Next, the researchers plan to use their imaging tools to visualize the
connections between individual neurons in the visual cortex and to examine how
these connections change when the brain “sees” an image.
“We were chasing this 99 percent of unexplored brain activity and
discovered that there’s so much richness in the visual cortex that nobody knew
anything about,” Andermann said.
Whether daydreams in people involve similar activity patterns in the visual
cortex is an open question, and the answer will require additional experiments.
However, there is preliminary evidence that an analogous process occurs in
humans when they recall visual imagery.
Randy Buckner, the Sosland Family Professor of
Psychology and of Neuroscience at Harvard University, has shown that brain activity in the visual
cortex increases when people are asked to recall an image in
detail. Other studies have recorded flurries of electrical activity in the
visual cortex and the hippocampus during such recall.
For the researchers, the results of their study and others suggest that it
may be important to make space for moments of quiet waking that lead to
daydreams. For a mouse, this may mean taking a pause from looking at a series
of images and, for a human, this could mean taking a break from scrolling on a
smartphone.
“We feel pretty confident that if you never give yourself any awake
downtime, you’re not going to have as many of these daydream events, which may
be important for brain plasticity,” Andermann said.
Source: https://hms.harvard.edu/news/what-happens-brain-while-daydreaming
Source: What Happens in the Brain While Daydreaming? – Scents of Science (myfusimotors.com)
No comments:
Post a Comment