When we use
our working memory, we temporarily retain information in our brain. For
instance, you are able to comprehend this sentence because you are briefly
storing in working memory each of the words you are reading until you put them
together to form the meaning of the sentence. The importance of working memory
to many of our cognitive abilities is well known, but less clear are the
neurological machinations driving this process.
A team of researchers has now demonstrated that the
key to understanding working memory relies
not only on what one is storing in memory, but also why. This is the
"working" part of working memory, which emphasizes the purpose of
storing something in the first place.
"We
now know that our visual memories are not simply what one has just seen, but,
instead, are the result of the neural codes dynamically evolving to incorporate
how you intend to use that information in the future," explains Clayton
Curtis, a professor of psychology and neural science at New York University and
the senior author of the paper, which appears in the journal Current Biology.
Specifically,
the study focuses on both how we store the visual properties of our memories in
the occipital lobe, where our visual system resides, and on how the neural
codes that store those memories change over time as people begin to prepare a
response that depends on the memory. In the study, the response simply required
people to look where they remembered an object that disappeared several seconds
ago.
"The
research makes it clear that memory codes can simultaneously contain
information about what we remember seeing and about the future behavior that
depends on those visual memories," notes Hsin-Hung Li, a New York
University postdoctoral researcher and the paper's lead author. "This
means the neural dynamics driving our working memory
result from reformatting memories into forms that are closer to later behaviors
that rely on visual memories."
Textbook
theories state that the storage codes for our working memory are stable over
time. This means that the pattern of neural activity that stores a given visual
memory is the same as when it was first seen and encoded—whether it is a second
later or 10 seconds later. These patterns of neural activity store visual memories,
providing a bridge across time between a past stimulus and a future memory
guided response.
This video of neural activity in primary visual
cortex (V1; left) provides evidence that the neural activity during a spatial
working memory task dynamically evolves from representing the memorized
location (pink circle) to representing the memory guided saccade that will
shift gaze from central fixation (pink cross) to memorized target location
(pink circle). Activity in other areas, like V3AB (right), only encode the past
target location with little dynamics. Credit: Clayton Curtis and Hsin-Hung Li
However, recent studies of animals indicate that these neural patterns are
much more dynamic—in fact, the memory codes are not stable and, instead, appear
to change over time in puzzling ways.
To explore this, Li and Curtis, who previously uncovered how our
working memory is formatted in the brain, devised innovative methods to both
measure changing neural dynamics and critically make the dynamics
interpretable. To do so, they projected the complex neural measurements into a
simple 2D plane, like the screen of your laptop or smartphone.
The accompanying video depicts how the pattern of neural activity evolves
during a working memory trial. Initially, you can see a bump of activity
encoding the briefly presented visual target (pink circle) in both primary visual cortex (V1) and
a high-level visual area (V3AB). In V3AB, this bump remains at the target
location throughout the memory delay. However in V1, a line of activity evolves
during the delay between where the person is looking (pink cross) and where
they will move their eyes after the delay.
The researchers believe that this line reflects the trajectory of the shift
of gaze that is being rehearsed in people's minds, but has yet to be executed.
Although previous work had documented neural dynamics during working
memory, the reason for why these dynamics occur had remained unknown. These new
results help address this mystery. They indicate that the dynamics reflect
transformations of past sensory events— what we have just seen—into future
memory guided behaviors—what we might do with the memory.
"We've now shown that mnemonic codes can simultaneously contain
information about a past remembered stimulus and the subsequent behavior that
depends on that stimulus," observes Curtis. "The neural dynamics of
our working memory result from reformatting memories in order to align them
with how we use them in the future."
by New York University
Source: New research shows how the visual system contributes to memory (medicalxpress.com)
No comments:
Post a Comment