Credit: Current Biology (2024). DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2024.10.045
When
bats can't hear, new research finds that these hearing-dependent animals employ
a remarkable compensation strategy. They adapt immediately and robustly,
suggesting for the first time that bats' brains are hard-wired with an ability
to launch a Plan B in times of diminished hearing.
The Johns Hopkins University work, newly published in Current Biology, raises questions about
whether other animals and even humans might be capable of such deft
accommodations.
"Bats have this amazing flexible
adaptive behavior that they can employ anytime," said senior author
Cynthia F. Moss, a Johns Hopkins neuroscientist who studies bats. "Other
mammals and humans also have these adaptive circuits that they can use to help
make decisions and navigate their environment, but what's striking here is that
it's very fast, almost automatic."
All animals adapt in various ways as a
response to sensory deprivation. People at a loud bar, might lean in to better
hear what someone is saying. A dog might tilt its head toward a muted sound.
Here researchers wondered how hearing-dependent echolocating bats might adapt when a key auditory region in the brain was turned off.
When bats can't hear, new research finds that
these hearing-dependent animals employ a remarkable compensation strategy.
Compare how a bat with normal hearing and a bat that temporarily cannot hear
fly from a platform, down a corridor and through a window to claim a treat.
Credit: Johns Hopkins University
They trained bats to fly from a
platform, down a corridor, and through a window to get a treat. Researchers
then had the same bats repeat the task but with a critical auditory pathway in
the midbrain temporarily blocked. Disabling this brain region isn't like
plugging your ears; it's preventing most auditory signals from reaching the
deep brain. The drug-induced technique is reversible and lasts about 90
minutes.
With their hearing blocked, bats
were able to navigate the course surprisingly well, even on the first try. They
weren't as agile and ran into things, but every tested bat compensated
immediately and effectively.
"They struggled but
managed," Moss said.
The bats changed their flight path
and vocalizations. They flew lower, oriented themselves along walls and
increased both the number and length of their calls, which boosted the power of
echo signals they use for navigation.
"Echolocation acts like
strobes, so they were basically taking more snapshots to help them get the
missing information," said co-author Clarice A. Diebold, a former Johns
Hopkins graduate student who is now a postdoctoral student at Washington University
in St. Louis.
"We also found that they
broadened the bandwidth on these calls. These adaptations are very interesting
because we'd usually see them when bats are compensating for external noise but
this is an internal processing deficit."
Although the team repeated the
experiments, the compensation skills of the bats didn't improve over time. This
means the adaptation behaviors the bats employed weren't learned; they were
innate, latent and hard-wired into the bats' brain circuitry.
"It highlights how robust the
brain is to manipulation and external noise," said co-author Jennifer Lawlor, a postdoctoral
fellow at Johns Hopkins.
The team was surprised that the
bats could hear at all with this region of their brain disabled. They
believe bats either relied on a previously unknown auditory
pathway or that unaffected neurons might support hearing in previously unknown
ways.
"You'd think an animal
wouldn't be able to hear at all," Moss said. "But it suggests that
there might be multiple pathways for sound to travel to the auditory
cortex."
The team would next like to
determine to what degree the findings apply to other animals and humans.
"Can this work tell us
something about auditory processing and adaptive responses in humans?"
Moss asked. "Since no one has done this, we don't know. The findings raise
important questions that will be exciting to pursue in other research models."
Authors include Kathryne Allen, Grace Capshaw, Megan G. Humphrey, Diego Cintron-De Leon and Kishore V. Kuchibhotla, all of Johns Hopkins.
by Jill Rosen, Johns Hopkins University
Source: Bats employ instant compensation strategy when they can't hear, study shows
No comments:
Post a Comment